50 种常见的认知偏差中文版
- 基本归因错误:我们根据他人的个性或基本特征来判断他人,但我们根据情况来判断自己。
- 自私偏见:我们的失败是因情况而定的,但我们的成功是我们的责任。
- 群体内偏爱:我们偏爱群体内的人,而不是群体外的人。
- 跟风效应:想法、时尚和信念随着越来越多的人采用而不断增长。
- 群体迷思:由于渴望团队一致与和谐,我们会做出非理性的决定,通常是为了尽量减少冲突。
- 光环效应:如果你认为一个人具有积极的特质,那么这种积极的印象就会蔓延到他们的其他特质中。(这也适用于负面特征。)
- 道德运气:积极的结果会带来更好的道德地位;由于负面结果,道德地位会变得更差。
- 虚假共识:我们相信同意我们观点的人比实际情况多。
- 知识的诅咒:一旦我们知道某件事,我们就假设其他人也知道它。
- 聚光灯效应:我们高估了人们对我们的行为和外表的关注程度。
- 可用性启发式:我们依靠在做出判断时立即想到的例子。
- 防御性归因:作为一个暗自担心自己容易遭受严重事故的目击者,如果我们与受害者有联系,我们就会减少对受害者的责备。
- 公正世界假说:我们倾向于相信世界是公正的;因此,我们认为不公正行为是应得的。
- 朴素现实主义:我们相信我们观察到的是客观现实,而其他人是非理性的、无知的或有偏见的。
- 天真的犬儒主义:我们相信我们观察到了客观现实,并且其他人的自我中心偏见比他们实际的意图/行为更高。
- 福勒效应(又名巴纳姆效应):我们很容易将我们的个性归因于模糊的陈述,即使它们可以适用于广泛的人。
- 邓宁-克鲁格效应:你知道的越少,你就越自信。你知道的越多,你就越不自信。
- 锚定:我们在做出决策时严重依赖引入的第一条信息。
- 自动化偏见:我们依赖自动化系统,有时过于相信自动纠正实际正确的决策。
- 谷歌效应(又名数字失忆症):我们往往会忘记在搜索引擎中轻松查找的信息。
- 反应:我们的做法与别人告诉我们的相反,尤其是当我们意识到个人自由受到威胁时。
- 确认偏差:我们倾向于寻找并记住能够证实我们看法的信息。
- 适得其反效应:反驳证据有时会产生不必要的效果,从而证实我们的信念。
- 第三人称效应:我们相信其他人比我们自己更容易受到大众媒体消费的影响。
- 信念偏差:我们判断一个论点的强度,不是看它对结论的支持程度,而是看这个结论在我们心目中的合理程度。
- 可用性级联:与我们对社会接受的需求相关,集体信仰通过公开重复获得更多可信度。
- 衰落主义:我们倾向于将过去浪漫化,并对未来持消极态度,认为社会/机构总体上正在衰落。
- 现状偏见:我们倾向于希望事情保持不变;相对于基线的变化被视为损失。
- 沉没成本谬误(又名承诺升级):即使我们面临负面结果,我们也会在那些让我们付出代价的事情上投入更多,而不是改变我们的投资。
- 赌徒谬误:我们认为未来的可能性受到过去事件的影响。
- 零风险偏差:我们更愿意将小风险降低到零,即使我们可以通过另一种选择整体降低更多风险。
- 框架效应:我们经常从相同的信息中得出不同的结论,具体取决于它的呈现方式。
- 刻板印象:我们普遍相信一个群体的成员将具有某些特征,尽管没有关于个人的信息。
- 外群体同质性偏见:我们认为外群体成员是同质的,而我们自己的内群体成员则更加多样化。
- 权威偏见:我们信任权威人物的观点,也更容易受到其影响。
- 安慰剂效应:如果我们相信某种治疗有效,它通常会产生很小的生理效应。
- 幸存者偏差:我们倾向于关注那些在过程中幸存下来的事情,而忽视那些失败的事情。
- 精神过速:我们对时间的感知取决于创伤、吸毒和体力消耗。
- 琐碎法则(又名“自行车脱落”):我们对琐碎问题给予不成比例的重视,同时往往避免更复杂的问题。
- 蔡加尼克效应:我们对未完成的任务的记忆多于已完成的任务。
- 宜家效应:我们对自己部分创造的事物给予更高的价值。
- 本·富兰克林效应:我们喜欢帮忙;如果我们已经帮过某人一个忙,那么我们更有可能再帮别人一个忙,而不是接受过那个人的帮忙。
- 旁观者效应:周围的人越多,我们帮助受害者的可能性就越小。
- 暗示性:我们,尤其是孩子,有时会将提问者提出的想法误认为是记忆。
- 错误记忆:我们将想象误认为是真实记忆。
- 密码记忆症:我们将真实的记忆误认为是想象。
- 聚类错觉:我们在随机数据中发现模式和“聚类”。
- 悲观偏见:我们有时会高估不良结果的可能性。
- 乐观偏见:我们有时对好的结果过于乐观。
- 盲点偏见:我们不认为自己有偏见,而且我们更多地看到别人而不是我们自己。
50 种常见的认知偏差英文原版
- fundamental attribution error: we judge others on their personality or fundamental character, but we judge ourselves on the situation.
- self-serving bias: our failures are situational, but our successes are our responsibility.
- in-group favoritism: we favor people who are in our in-group as opposed to an out-group.
- bandwagon effect: ideas, fads, and beliefs grow as more people adopt them.
- groupthink: due to a desire for conformity and harmony in the group, we make irrational decisions, often to minimize conflict.
- halo effect: if you see a person as having a positive trait, that positive impression will spill over into their other traits. (this also works for negative traits.)
- moral luck: better moral standing happens due to a positive outcome; worse moral standing happens due to a negative outcome.
- false consensus: we believe more people agree with us than is actually the case.
- curse of knowledge: once we know something, we assume everyone else knows it, too.
- spotlight effect: we overestimate how much people are paying attention to our behavior and appearance.
- availability heuristic: we rely on immediate examples that come to mind while making judgments.
- defensive attribution: as a witness who secretly fears being vulnerable to a serious mishap, we will blame the victim less if we relate to the victim.
- just-world hypothesis: we tend to believe the world is just; therefore, we assume acts of injustice are deserved.
- naïve realism: we believe that we observe objective reality and that other people are irrational, uninformed, or biased.
- naïve cynicism: we believe that we observe objective reality and that other people have a higher egocentric bias than they actually do in their intentions/actions.
- forer effect (aka barnum effect): we easily attribute our personalities to vague statements, even if they can apply to a wide range of people.
- dunning-kruger effect: the less you know, the more confident you are. the more you know, the less confident you are.
- anchoring: we rely heavily on the first piece of information introduced when making decisions.
- automation bias: we rely on automated systems, sometimes trusting too much in the automated correction of actually correct decisions.
- google effect (aka digital amnesia): we tend to forget information that’s easily looked up in search engines.
- reactance: we do the opposite of what we’re told, especially when we perceive threats to personal freedoms.
- confirmation bias: we tend to find and remember information that confirms our perceptions.
- backfire effect: disproving evidence sometimes has the unwarranted effect of confirming our beliefs.
- third-person effect: we believe that others are more affected by mass media consumption than we ourselves are.
- belief bias: we judge an argument’s strength not by how strongly it supports the conclusion but how plausible the conclusion is in our own minds.
- availability cascade: tied to our need for social acceptance, collective beliefs gain more plausibility through public repetition.
- declinism: we tent to romanticize the past and view the future negatively, believing that societies/institutions are by and large in decline.
- status quo bias: we tend to prefer things to stay the same; changes from the baseline are considered to be a loss.
- sunk cost fallacy (aka escalation of commitment): we invest more in things that have cost us something rather than altering our investments, even if we face negative outcomes.
- gambler’s fallacy: we think future possibilities are affected by past events.
- zero-risk bias: we prefer to reduce small risks to zero, even if we can reduce more risk overall with another option.
- framing effect: we often draw different conclusions from the same information depending on how it’s presented.
- stereotyping: we adopt generalized beliefs that members of a group will have certain characteristics, despite not having information about the individual.
- outgroup homogeneity bias: we perceive out-group members as homogeneous and our own in-groups as more diverse.
- authority bias: we trust and are more often influenced by the opinions of authority figures.
- placebo effect: if we believe a treatment will work, it often will have a small physiological effect.
- survivorship bias: we tend to focus on those things that survived a process and overlook ones that failed.
- tachypsychia: our perceptions of time shift depending on trauma, drug use, and physical exertion.
- law of triviality (aka “bike-shedding”): we give disproportionate weight to trivial issues, often while avoiding more complex issues.
- zeigarnik effect: we remember incomplete tasks more than completed ones.
- ikea effect: we place higher value on things we partially created ourselves.
- ben franklin effect: we like doing favors; we are more likely to do another favor for someone if we’ve already done a favor for them than if we had received a favor from that person.
- bystander effect: the more other people are around, the less likely we are to help a victim.
- suggestibility: we, especially children, sometimes mistake ideas suggested by a questioner for memories.
- false memory: we mistake imagination for real memories.
- cryptomnesia: we mistake real memories for imagination.
- clustering illusion: we find patterns and “clusters” in random data.
- pessimism bias: we sometimes overestimate the likelihood of bad outcomes.
- optimism bias: we sometimes are over-optimistic about good outcomes.
- blind spot bias: we don’t think we have bias, and we see it others more than ourselves.
历史上的今天:
- 2022: 微语录精选:我的一些美好品质
- 2022: 游戏推荐[pc端游戏]橡胶强盗|rubber bandits
- 2022:
- 2022:
- 2022: 近期线上高分电影剧集(开播或完结)大合集,让11月有一点精彩!